Internet Addiction Prevalence

Internet addiction prevalence (PINTA)

Report to the Federal Minis­try of Health, Germany

Pro­ject dura­ti­on: Novem­ber 15, 2010 to Febru­a­ry 14, 2011
Fun­ding code: Chap­ter 15 02 Tit­le 684 69
Fun­ding amount: € 14,580

Hans-Jür­gen Rumpf, Chris­ti­an Mey­er, Anja Kreu­zer & Ulrich John

Fun­ding amount: € 14,580

Ad Ver­mulst (Depart­ment of Deve­lo­p­men­tal Psy­cho­pa­tho­lo­gy, Beha­viou­ral Sci­ence Insti­tu­te, Rad­boud Uni­ver­si­ty Nij­me­gen, Nie­der­lan­de)
Gert-Jan Mer­keerk (IVO Addic­tion Rese­arch Insti­tu­te, Rot­ter­dam, Niederlande)

Manage­ment and con­ta­ct address: PD Dr. Hans-Jür­gen Rumpf
Uni­ver­si­ty of Lübeck, Cli­nic for Psych­ia­try and Psy­cho­the­ra­py, Rat­ze­bur­ger Allee 160, 23538
Lübeck, Tel. 0451/5002871, email: hans-juergen.rumpf@psychiatrie.uk-sh.de
Greifs­wald and Lübeck, 31.05.2011

Table of contents

  1. Con­clu­si­on
  2. Intro­duc­tion
    1.1 Star­ting point of the pro­ject
    1.2 Sta­te of rese­arch
    1.3 Aim of the PINTA stu­dy
    1.4 Pro­ject struc­tu­re, struc­tures and responsibilities
  3. Sur­vey and eva­lua­ti­on metho­do­lo­gy
    2.1 sam­ple
    2.1.1 Land­li­ne sam­ple
    2.1.2 Mobi­le-only sam­ple
    2.2 Sur­vey pro­ce­du­re
    2.3 Sta­tis­ti­cal Ana­ly­sis
    2.3.1 Pro­ce­du­re for esti­ma­ting pre­va­lence
    2.3.2 Weigh­t­ing of the tele­pho­ne sam­ple and con­si­de­ra­ti­on of the sam­ple design
    in data evaluation
  4. Imple­men­ta­ti­on, work plan and schedule
  5. Results
    4.1 Pre­va­lence esti­ma­te based on the cut-off
    4.2 Pre­va­lence Esti­ma­ti­on Based on Latent Class Ana­ly­sis (LCA)
  6. Dis­cus­sion of the results
  7. Gen­der Main­strea­ming Aspects
  8. Over­all assessment
  9. Dis­se­mi­na­ti­on and publi­ci­ty of the pro­ject results
  10. Uti­liz­a­ti­on of the pro­ject results (sus­taina­bi­li­ty / trans­fer potential)
  11. List of publications
  12. Lite­ra­tu­re

0. Summary

Back­ground: The data on the pre­va­lence of inter­net addic­tion are defi­ci­ent. The avail­ab­le fin­dings for Ger­ma­ny show metho­do­lo­gi­cal defi­ci­en­ci­es; in par­ti­cu­lar, they are not based on repre­sen­ta­ti­ve sam­ples. The pre­sent ana­ly­sis can fall back on a lar­ge and repre­sen­ta­ti­ve sam­ple that was recrui­ted as part of the stu­dy Patho­lo­gi­cal Gamb­ling and Epi­de­mio­lo­gy (PAGE). In this pro­ject, Inter­net addic­tion was also recor­ded as a comor­bi­di­ty using the Com­pul­si­ve Inter­net Use Sca­le (CIUS).

Method: The sam­ple con­sis­ted of 15,024 peop­le aged 14–64 who were inter­view­ed by tele­pho­ne and of whom 1,001 could only be reached via mobi­le pho­ne and not via land­li­ne. In addi­ti­on to a pro­por­tio­nal ran­dom sam­pling approach, the repre­sen­ta­ti­ve­ness was ensu­red through exten­si­ve weightin­gs. The pre­va­lence was esti­ma­ted in the PINTA stu­dy via the CIUS, in which 1. used a cut-off from ano­t­her stu­dy and 2. A latent class ana­ly­sis was cal­cu­la­ted on the basis of the CIUS items.

Results: Based on the cut-off of 28, the esti­ma­ted pre­va­lence for inter­net addic­tion is 1.5% (women 1.3%, men 1.7%). When using the LCA, the rates are slight­ly lower at 1% (women 0.8%, men 1.2%). In the 14–24 age group, the pre­va­lence incre­a­ses to 2.4% (women 2.5%, men 2.5%). If only 14–16 year olds are con­si­de­red, 4.0% are inter­net addicts (women 4.9%, men 3.1%). The high pro­por­ti­on among young girls can be found in both metho­do­lo­gi­cal approa­ches. The girls and women (14–24 years old) who were con­spi­cuous main­ly use social net­works on the Inter­net (77.1% of addicts accord­ing to LCA) and rare­ly use online games (7.2%). The young men also use social net­works, albeit to a les­ser extent (64.8%), but more often online games (33.6%). With the help of the LCA, a fur­ther group with pro­ble­ma­tic inter­net use can be iden­ti­fied in addi­ti­on to the pres­um­a­b­ly addicts, which affects a total of 4.6% of the respondents (women 4.4%, men 4.9%). Here, too, the­re are high rates among young cohorts, and par­ti­cu­lar­ly among women.

Con­clu­si­on: Com­pa­red to an ear­lier esti­ma­te of 3.2% based on ano­t­her stu­dy, PINTA has lower, yet signi­fi­cant rates. The accu­ra­cy of the esti­ma­te is signi­fi­cant­ly impro­ved here on the basis of the repre­sen­ta­ti­ve sam­ple. The high pre­va­lence among girls and young women is striking. Fur­ther stu­dies with in-depth ana­ly­zes are necessary.

1 Introduction

1.1 Starting point of the project‌

Inter­net addic­tion is a still litt­le rese­ar­ched form of non-sub­s­tance addic­tion. Much atten­ti­on is cur­r­ent­ly being paid to it, inclu­ding: becau­se it could be a pro­blem of incre­a­sing impor­t­ance. So far it has not been cla­ri­fied whe­ther (1) addic­tion pro­blems with Inter­net use repre­sent a signi­fi­cant dis­or­der with cli­ni­cal rele­van­ce and (2) whe­ther their pre­va­lence in the popu­la­ti­on is of a magnitu­de that requi­res federal poli­ti­cal action. Howe­ver, due to the lack of suf­fi­ci­ent­ly valid data, the­re have been no mea­ning­ful stu­dies of the pro­blem so far.

1.2 State of research‌

Inter­na­tio­nal­ly, pre­va­lence rates bet­ween 1 and 14% can be found (Chris­ta­kis, 2010). The data on the fre­quen­cy of inter­net addic­tion inter­na­tio­nal­ly and for Ger­ma­ny have been view­ed and sum­ma­ri­zed in a pro­ject fun­ded by the Federal Minis­try of Health (BMG) (Peter­sen et al., 2010). The aut­hors con­clu­de that the­re are a num­ber of metho­do­lo­gi­cal pro­blems so that only preli­mi­na­ry esti­ma­tes are pos­si­ble. The main pro­blems are that in many cases the sam­ples are casu­al and can­not claim to be repre­sen­ta­ti­ve, and that sur­vey methods have been used that have not been vali­da­ted. In addi­ti­on, the­re is cur­r­ent­ly no uni­form defi­ni­ti­on of inter­net addic­tion (Byun et al., 2009).

The only pre­vious stu­dy for Ger­ma­ny that also cove­r­ed the area of adults comes from Hahn and Jeru­sa­lem (2001). On the basis of an online sam­ple of more than 7,000 peop­le, the pre­va­lence is 3.2% across all age groups; the pro­por­ti­on rises among youn­ger sub­jects. Boys under the age of 18 were about twice as likely to be affec­ted com­pa­red to fema­le par­ti­ci­pants, who were, howe­ver, over­all under­re­pre­sen­ted in the stu­dy. Howe­ver, a lar­ge pro­por­ti­on of the stu­dies on inter­net addic­tion only refer to ado­lescents. Based on a sur­vey in schools, Meix­ner (2010) reports a fre­quen­cy of 1.4% among 12 to 25-year-olds. Fur­ther stu­dies are limi­ted to com­pu­ter game beha­vi­or. A repre­sen­ta­ti­ve stu­dent sur­vey found a rate of 1.7% for this (Reh­bein, Klei­mann & Mos­s­le, 2010).
Sin­ce the detail­ed review by Peter­sen and Tho­ma­si­us (2010) on patho­lo­gi­cal Inter­net use in Ger­ma­ny, ano­t­her sur­vey has been car­ri­ed out at the Uni­ver­si­ty of Mainz on 2,512 peop­le inclu­ding adult­hood with the aim of esti­ma­ting the pre­va­lence. Howe­ver, the data have not yet been publis­hed.
In sum­ma­ry, the data situa­ti­on with regard to the fre­quen­cy of pro­ble­ma­tic Inter­net use or Inter­net depen­den­cy in Ger­ma­ny is cur­r­ent­ly incom­ple­te. The pre­vious stu­dies have exami­ned sub­po­pu­la­ti­ons or did not show a repre­sen­ta­ti­ve basis.

1.3 Aim of the PINTA study‌

The aim of PINTA was to pro­vi­de the most reli­able figu­res pos­si­ble for the extent of inter­net addic­tion by over­co­m­ing two metho­do­lo­gi­cal weak­nes­ses of pre­vious stu­dies: a) by the inclu­si­on of both ado­lescents and adults and b) by ensu­ring repre­sen­ta­ti­ve­ness. For the assess­ment of Inter­net addic­tion in Ger­ma­ny, the ana­ly­sis of a data set from the stu­dy “Patho­lo­gi­cal Gamb­ling and Epi­de­mio­lo­gy (PAGE)” was car­ri­ed out. In this stu­dy, inter­net addic­tion was recor­ded as a comor­bi­di­ty of patho­lo­gi­cal gamb­ling. Sin­ce PAGE offers a repre­sen­ta­ti­ve and lar­ge sam­ple in the age group 14–64, a more pre­cise esti­ma­te of the pre­va­lence was possible.

1.4 Project structure, structures and responsibilities‌

The pro­ject was led by the Uni­ver­si­ty of Lübeck. The­re was clo­se coope­ra­ti­on with the Insti­tu­te for Epi­de­mio­lo­gy and Social Medi­ci­ne of the Uni­ver­si­ty Medi­ci­ne in Greifs­wald. This was ensu­red struc­tu­ral­ly through tele­pho­ne con­fe­ren­ces and regu­lar bila­te­ral tele­pho­ne con­ta­cts.
In order to obtain addi­tio­nal exper­ti­se in the field of inter­net addic­tion, Dr. Gert-Jan Meer­kerk (IVO Addic­tion Rese­arch Insti­tu­te, Rot­ter­dam, Nether­lands). For sta­tis­ti­cal ana­ly­sis, the pro­ject was con­ti­nued by Dr. Ad A. Ver­mulst (Depart­ment of Deve­lo­p­men­tal Psy­cho­pa­tho­lo­gy, Beha­vio­ral Sci­ence Insti­tu­te, Rad­boud Uni­ver­si­ty Nij­me­gen, Nether­lands), who is an expert in the cal­cu­la­ti­on of latent class ana­ly­zes. Two sci­en­ti­fic employees were hired who had alrea­dy worked on the PAGE stu­dy, so that syn­er­gies could be crea­ted here.

2. Survey and evaluation methodology‌

An ana­ly­sis of the data from the PAGE pro­ject was car­ri­ed out. PAGE was fun­ded by the federal sta­tes as part of the Sta­te Trea­ty on Gamb­ling and car­ri­ed out from Decem­ber 1st, 2009 to Febru­a­ry 28th, 2011. The metho­do­lo­gy and the first results are avail­ab­le in the form of a final report (Mey­er et al., 2011). PAGE’s mul­ti­modal recrui­t­ing approach inclu­ded, among other things, a nati­on­wi­de tele­pho­ne sur­vey. This forms the basis for the data ana­ly­sis car­ri­ed out in PINTA.

2.1 Sample‌

Two sam­ples were taken for the tele­pho­ne sur­vey. In addi­ti­on to a ran­dom selec­tion of land­li­ne num­bers, peop­le who can only be reached via mobi­le pho­nes were also recrui­ted. This strand is of par­ti­cu­lar impor­t­ance becau­se the pro­por­ti­on of peop­le who can only be reached this way is signi­fi­cant and con­ti­nues to rise. Evi­dence emer­ges that this is a popu­la­ti­on with spe­ci­fic cha­rac­te­ris­tics. For examp­le, the pre­va­lence of patho­lo­gi­cal gamb­ling is incre­a­sed in this group (Mey­er et al., 2011). A com­pu­ter-aided tele­pho­ne inter­view (CATI) of 15 minu­tes on average was car­ri­ed out with all par­ti­ci­pants by trai­ned interviewers.

2.1.1 Fixed line sample‌

The sam­pling was car­ri­ed out in a mul­ti-sta­ge pro­ce­du­re by infas. A detail­ed descrip­ti­on can be found in the cor­re­spon­ding method report (Hess, Stein­we­de, Gil­berg & Kleud­gen, 2011), which is avail­ab­le on request from the aut­hors of this report. In the first sta­ge, pri­ma­ry sam­pling units (PSU) were drawn. The pro­ba­bi­li­ty of selec­tion of the muni­ci­pa­li­ties (sam­ple points) was pro­por­tio­nal to the resi­dent popu­la­ti­on in the tar­get group. Ins­ge­samt wur­den 53 Sam­ple Points in 52 Gemein­den bestimmt (Ber­lin war mit zwei Sam­ple Points ver­tre­ten). For the ran­dom selec­tion of the PSUs, stra­ti­fi­ca­ti­ons were made accord­ing to the federal sta­tes, admi­nis­tra­ti­ve districts and coun­ties as well as the slot machi­ne den­si­ty. The selec­ted muni­ci­pa­li­ties are shown in Figu­re 1.

In a second step, the house­holds were deter­mi­ned on the basis of tele­pho­ne num­bers. The­se Secon­da­ry Sam­pling Units (SSUs) were assi­gned to the muni­ci­pa­li­ties via the area codes. In each muni­ci­pa­li­ty, 5,800 tele­pho­ne num­bers were drawn for the gross sam­ple. In the third step, the tar­get per­sons were deter­mi­ned (Third Sam­pling Unit, TSU). If the­re was more than one per­son belon­ging to the tar­get group (ages 14 to 64), the per­son who was last born was chosen.

Figu­re 1: Sam­ple points (repre­sen­ted by blue mar­kings; pink mar­kings repre­sent the­ra­py faci­li­ties for patho­lo­gi­cal gambling)

Bet­ween June 7 and Octo­ber 22, 2010, a total of 14,022 tele­pho­ne inter­views from the fixed net­work sam­ple were car­ri­ed out. Of 26,736 house­holds in which a tar­get per­son aged bet­ween 14 and 64 lived, an inter­view could be car­ri­ed out with 52.4% of the tar­get per­sons after using the last bir­th­day ques­ti­on, 38.9% refu­sed to take part in the sur­vey. 8.7% of the tar­get per­sons did not take part becau­se the con­ta­ct per­son refu­sed access (4.6%), becau­se they were too serious­ly ill for a sur­vey (1.4%) or becau­se they could not be reached (2.7%; Hess & Stein­we­de, 2011).

2.1.2 Mobile-only sample‌

The ran­dom sam­ple of peop­le who can­not be reached via the fixed net­work, but only via mobi­le pho­ne, was drawn nati­on­wi­de becau­se of the unrea­liz­ab­le regio­nal allo­ca­ti­on. The tar­get group were again peop­le bet­ween the ages of 14 and 64 years. From the ran­dom­ly drawn pho­ne num­bers, only tho­se peop­le who can only be reached via a mobi­le pho­ne were selec­ted via a scree­ning.
A total of 1,001 tele­pho­ne inter­views were car­ri­ed out bet­ween Novem­ber 22, 2010 and Febru­a­ry 1, 2011 in the mobi­le-only sam­ple. To this end, 13,273 peop­le had to be scree­n­ed to ensu­re that they could only be reached by mobi­le pho­ne. Of the 1,767 poten­ti­al tar­get per­sons iden­ti­fied, 747 per­sons (42.3%) refu­sed to take part. 7 peop­le (0.4%) were unab­le to take part in the tele­pho­ne inter­view due to ill­ness or disa­bi­li­ty. 12 peop­le (0.7%) drop­ped out due to insuf­fi­ci­ent know­ledge of Ger­man (Hess & Stein­we­de, 2011).

2.2 Survey procedure‌

Im Mit­tel­punkt der Prä­va­lenz­schät­zung für patho­lo­gi­schen Inter­net­ge­brauch stand die Com­pul­si­ve Inter­net Use Sca­le (CIUS; Meer­kerk, Van Den Eijn­den, Ver­mulst & Gar­ret­sen, 2009), ein Fra­ge­bo­gen­ver­fah­ren zur Erfas­sung von Merk­ma­len der Inter­net­ab­hän­gig­keit. Your 14 items have a five-level ans­wer for­mat (Figu­re 2), with bet­ween 0 and 56 points being achie­ved. The method was deve­lo­ped in several sub­sam­ples and shows a one-fac­to­ri­al struc­tu­re throughout. The­re are also data from the gene­ral popu­la­ti­on avail­ab­le, which speaks in favor of choo­sing this method for use in epi­de­mio­lo­gi­cal sur­veys. The Cronbach’s alpha as a mea­su­re of inter­nal con­sis­ten­cy was .89 and indi­ca­tes good relia­bi­li­ty. A con­ver­gent vali­di­ty with simi­lar pro­ce­du­res was shown. The­re is cur­r­ent­ly no recom­men­ded cut-off based on a broad data­ba­se. Initi­al indi­ca­ti­ons sug­gest a thres­hold value of 28 (see 3.3.1; Van Rooij, Schoe­n­ma­kers, Ver­mulst, Van Den Eijn­den & Van De Mheen, 2011).

Figu­re 2: Items of the CIUS (ans­wer cate­go­ries: never, rare­ly, some­ti­mes, often, very often)

  1. How often do you find it dif­fi­cult to stop using the inter­net while online?
  2. How often do you con­ti­nue to use the inter­net when you wan­ted to stop?
  3. How often do other peop­le, e.g. your part­ner, child­ren, par­ents or friends tell you that you
  4. should use the inter­net less?
  5. How often do you pre­fer to use the inter­net ins­tead of spen­ding time with others, e.g. with your partner,
  6. Child­ren, par­ents, friends?
  7. How often do you sleep too litt­le becau­se of the internet?
  8. How often do you think of the inter­net when you are not online?
  9. How often do you look for­ward to your next inter­net session?
  10. How often do you think about spen­ding less time online?
  11. How many times have you tried unsuc­cess­ful­ly to spend less time online?
  12. How often do you rush to do your cho­res at home so you can get on the inter­net sooner?
  13. How often do you neglect your ever­y­day respon­si­bi­li­ties (work, school, fami­ly life) because
  14. you pre­fer to go online?
  15. How often do you go online when you feel down?
  16. How often do you use the inter­net to escape your worries or
  17. relie­ve a nega­ti­ve mood?
  18. How often do you feel rest­less, frus­tra­ted, or irri­ta­ble when you can­not use the internet?

All per­sons who sta­ted that they had used the Inter­net for pri­va­te pur­po­ses eit­her for at least one hour on a week­day or one day on the wee­kend were asked the ques­ti­ons of the CIUS. The pro­ce­du­re was used in con­junc­tion with other sur­vey instru­ments. In terms of con­tent, the focus was on the record­ing of gamb­ling beha­vi­or and patho­lo­gi­cal gamb­ling. In the order in which they were pre­sen­ted, a pro­ce­du­re for record­ing social capi­tal was first used, fol­lo­wed by CIUS, the inter­view on gamb­ling and socio-demo­gra­phic ques­ti­ons. Social capi­tal was recor­ded using 12 ques­ti­ons about par­ti­ci­pa­ti­on in social events in the last 12 mon­ths (cine­ma, spor­ting event, art exhi­bi­ti­on, fur­ther edu­ca­ti­on etc .; Han­son, Öster­gren, Elm­stahl, Isaacs­son & Ran­s­tam, 1997). In addi­ti­on to this infor­ma­ti­on on the “Social Par­ti­ci­pa­ti­on” con­struct, the pro­ce­du­re also inclu­des a ques­ti­on as to the extent to which the­re is a gene­ral fee­ling of being able to trust other peop­le (“Trust”). Patho­lo­gi­cal gamb­ling was recor­ded using the gamb­ling sec­tion of the Com­po­si­te Inter­na­tio­nal Dia­gnostic Inter­view (CIDI) (WHO, 2009). It was also asked what acti­vi­ties more than 50% of the time is spent on the Inter­net. The free text infor­ma­ti­on has been com­bi­ned into main cate­go­ries. In the case of mul­ti­ple respon­ses, the first respon­se was evaluated.

2.3 Statistical Analysis‌

2.3.1 Procedure for estimating prevalence‌

The fre­quen­cy of inter­net addic­tion was esti­ma­ted in two ways:

  1. A cut-off was used for the scree­ning ques­ti­onn­aire CIUS, which was taken from a dif­fe­rent sam­ple (Van Rooij et al., 2011). This limit comes from two ran­dom sam­ples of 13 to 16 year old stu­dents (n = 1,572 / 1,476). The aim of the stu­dy was to iden­ti­fy a sus­pi­cious group with addic­tion to online video games. Such a group was iden­ti­fied by means of a latent class ana­ly­sis (LCA) based on the CIUS. A cut-off of 28 or more points was found to be favor­able. With the help of this thres­hold value, a rough esti­ma­te of Inter­net depen­den­cy could be made in PAGE.
  2. With the help of a modern sta­tis­ti­cal method, the latent class ana­ly­sis (LCA), which is par­ti­cu­lar­ly infor­ma­ti­ve for our pur­po­se, a group was iden­ti­fied which, based on its respon­se pat­tern, can be view­ed as likely depen­dent. LCA is a method for iden­ti­fy­ing signi­fi­cant groups of peop­le, who­se respon­se beha­vi­or is simi­lar. The cal­cu­la­ti­on was done with Mplus 5.1. (Muthén & Muthén, 1998). Several models were cal­cu­la­ted to deter­mi­ne a sub­group showing cha­rac­te­ris­tics of Inter­net addic­tion and a num­ber of good­ness-of-fit mea­su­res were used to select a model. The­se mea­su­res inclu­ded: Baye­si­an Infor­ma­ti­on Cri­ter­ion (BIC) ‑value (low values indi­ca­te bet­ter fit), Entro­py mea­su­re (hig­her values indi­ca­te bet­ter fit), Vuong-Lo-Men­dell-Rubin likeli­hood ratio test, and adjus­ted Lo-Men­dell ‑Rubin Likeli­hood Ratio Test (a p‑value <.05 indi­ca­tes that the model is bet­ter than the pre­vious one). The boot­strap­ped likeli­hood ratio test (BLRT) was not app­li­ca­ble becau­se it can­not be cal­cu­la­ted with weigh­ted data. Ano­t­her important cri­ter­ion is the use­ful­ness of the model based on theo­re­ti­cal or prac­ti­cal con­si­de­ra­ti­ons. The cha­rac­te­ris­tics of the clas­ses are che­cked by means of infe­ren­ti­al sta­tis­ti­cal com­pa­ri­sons with the other clas­ses. The LCA cal­cu­la­ti­ons were done by Dr. Ad A. Ver­mulst, Depart­ment of Deve­lo­p­men­tal Psy­cho­pa­tho­lo­gy, Beha­vio­ral Sci­ence Insti­tu­te, Rad­boud Uni­ver­si­ty Nij­me­gen, Holland.

2.3.2 Weighting of the telephone sample and consideration of the sample design in the data evaluation‌

All data were ana­ly­zed using sam­ple weights. Sin­ce esti­ma­tes based on the Ger­man popu­la­ti­on should be made on the basis of the tele­pho­ne sam­ple, spe­cial atten­ti­on was paid to the deve­lo­p­ment of ade­qua­te weigh­t­ing varia­bles. The weights were deve­lo­ped sepa­r­ate­ly for the land­li­ne and mobi­le net­work sam­ples. First, so-cal­led design weights were deter­mi­ned to com­pen­sa­te for dif­fe­rent selec­tion pro­ba­bi­li­ties through the sam­ple design. The dis­tor­ting effects of the mul­ti-sta­ge drawing pro­cess were com­pen­sa­ted for. For examp­le, house­holds that can be reached via several tele­pho­ne con­nec­tions are more likely to be selec­ted than house­holds with only one con­nec­tion. By con­trast, peop­le who live in mul­ti-per­son house­holds have a lower selec­tion pro­ba­bi­li­ty than peop­le in sin­gle house­holds due to the design, as only one per­son in the spe­ci­fied age ran­ge per house­hold was sur­vey­ed. Final­ly, the mobi­le pho­ne and land­li­ne pho­ne sam­ples had to be mer­ged. The weigh­t­ing had to be used to repro­du­ce the pro­por­ti­on of peop­le in the popu­la­ti­on with a land­li­ne con­nec­tion or who can only be reached via a mobi­le pho­ne con­nec­tion (“Mobi­le-Onlys”) in the sam­ple. Based on cur­rent fin­dings, a pro­por­ti­on of “mobi­le-onlys” in the 14 to 64 year old popu­la­ti­on of 14% (infas social rese­arch; unpu­blis­hed data) is assumed.

A second weigh­t­ing step con­sis­ted of balan­cing the dif­fe­rent wil­ling­ness to par­ti­ci­pa­te in dif­fe­rent socio-demo­gra­phi­cal­ly defi­ned popu­la­ti­on groups on the basis of exis­ting mar­gi­nal dis­tri­bu­ti­ons from offi­cial sta­tis­tics (redress­ment). Due to the signi­fi­cant asso­cia­ti­on of gamb­ling pro­blems with various social indi­ca­tors accord­ing to pre­vious fin­dings, in order to avoid dis­tor­ted pre­va­lence esti­ma­tes it was of gre­at impor­t­ance, in addi­ti­on to age and gen­der dis­tri­bu­ti­ons, to adapt the cha­rac­te­ris­tics of schoo­ling, unem­ploy­ment and migra­ti­on back­ground to the popu­la­ti­on. Basi­cal­ly, the expan­si­on of the cha­rac­te­ris­tics for the redress­ment is accom­pa­nied by a reduc­tion in the effec­ti­ve num­ber of cases in the sam­ple and thus an incre­a­se in the sam­pling error. Thus, when selec­ting fea­tures to be con­si­de­red, a tra­de-off must be made bet­ween pos­si­ble dis­tor­ti­on and the accu­ra­cy of the point esti­ma­tes. Against this back­ground, the fol­lowing ana­ly­zes were based on a weigh­t­ing that inclu­des the social indi­ca­tors men­tio­ned, but not the cha­rac­te­ris­tic of the poli­ti­cal size class of the com­mu­ni­ty of resi­dence. A dis­tor­ti­on of the results is hard­ly to be expec­ted as the poli­ti­cal size class of the resi­den­ti­al com­mu­ni­ty was alrea­dy taken into account as a stra­ti­fi­ca­ti­on cri­ter­ion in the com­mu­ni­ty selec­tion and no signi­fi­cant con­nec­tion with the main inves­ti­ga­ti­on cri­ter­ion gamb­ling pro­blems can be assu­med.
For the infe­ren­ti­al sta­tis­ti­cal vali­da­ti­on of the fin­dings pre­sen­ted below, the sam­ple design was taken into account when esti­ma­ting the sam­pling errors, as far as metho­do­lo­gi­cal­ly pos­si­ble, sin­ce an ana­ly­sis using stan­dard methods, which assu­me a simp­le ran­dom sam­ple, would lead to signi­fi­cant distortions.

3. Implementation, work plan and schedule‌

Three mon­ths were plan­ned for the imple­men­ta­ti­on of the pro­ject. The work to be per­for­med inclu­ded the review of the lite­ra­tu­re, the pre­pa­ra­ti­on of the data sets, the data ana­ly­sis, publi­ca­ti­on and repor­ting. The work was delay­ed for two rea­sons: 1. The com­plex sta­tis­ti­cal pro­ce­du­re based on the LCA in coope­ra­ti­on with the rese­ar­chers from the Nether­lands requi­red a grea­ter amount of time, espe­cial­ly for the respec­ti­ve agree­ments and con­tent-rela­ted dis­cus­sions of the pro­ce­du­re and the results. 2. In the PAGE stu­dy, as part of the tele­pho­ne sur­vey, the inclu­si­on of peop­le who can only be reached via mobi­le pho­ne was made pos­si­ble. The cor­re­spon­ding sur­veys were not com­ple­ted until Febru­a­ry 1, 2011. Howe­ver, it was deci­ded to use this sub-sam­ple and also to inclu­de it in the ana­ly­zes becau­se it repres­ents a signi­fi­cant impro­ve­ment in the metho­do­lo­gy. In addi­ti­on to the plan­ned work, we have used the oppor­tu­ni­ty to pro­vi­de ser­vices bey­ond the scope of the resour­ces in this pro­ject. This expan­si­on of our ser­vices has signi­fi­cant­ly impro­ved the qua­li­ty of the results. After the end of the fun­ding peri­od, the work was con­ti­nued by the pro­ject manage­ment using its own resour­ces, so that it was com­ple­ted by the time the report was drawn up. A publi­ca­ti­on of the results in a spe­cia­list jour­nal is still pen­ding due to the delays.

4. Results‌

Of the 15,023 peop­le sur­vey­ed, 8,130 (54.1%) sta­ted that they had used the Inter­net for pri­va­te pur­po­ses eit­her for at least one hour on a week­day or one day on the wee­kend and recei­ved the ques­ti­ons from the CIUS. All of the fol­lowing ana­ly­zes have been car­ri­ed out on the basis of weigh­ted data, unless other­wi­se noted.

4.1 Prevalence estimate based on the cut-off‌

Based on the CIUS cut-off of 28, as descri­bed in the metho­do­lo­gy, the esti­ma­ted pre­va­lence of pro­bable inter­net addic­tion for the total sam­ple of 14- to 64-year-olds is 1.5% based on all par­ti­ci­pants (sub­jects, who were not asked about inter­net usa­ge due to the fil­ter are con­si­de­red incon­spi­cuous). Fin­dings by gen­der and the respec­ti­ve con­fi­dence inter­vals can be found in Table 1.

Table 1: Pre­va­lence esti­ma­te based on cut-off 28 of the CIUS, ages 14–64 (n = 15,023)


Pre­va­lence

(%)

Con­fi­dence interval

(%)

Total

1,5

1,3–1,7

women

1,3

1,0–1,7

men

1,7

1,3–2,1

If the youn­ger age group is con­si­de­red sepa­r­ate­ly, the pre­va­lence figu­res are hig­her and the­re is a shift wit­hin the sexes to a hig­her pro­por­ti­on among the fema­le par­ti­ci­pants (Tables 2 and 3).

Table 2: Pre­va­lence esti­ma­te based on cut-off 28 of the CIUS, ages 14–24 (n = 2,937)


Pre­va­lence

(%)

Con­fi­dence interval

(%)

Total

3,8

3,0–4,6

women

4,5

3,3–6,0

men

3,0

2,3–4,3

Table 3: Pre­va­lence esti­ma­te based on cut-off 28 of the CIUS, ages 14–16 (n = 693)


Pre­va­lence

(%)

Con­fi­dence interval

(%)

Total

6,3

4,6–8,4

women

8,6

5,5–13,0

men

4,1

2,6–6,3

If one con­si­ders the group of 14 to 24 year-olds sepa­ra­ted by sex, one can see for the suspect in the CIUS (pre­su­med inter­net addicts) that fema­le par­ti­ci­pants main­ly sta­ted social net­works as the first men­tio­ned main acti­vi­ty on the inter­net (81.4%; Table 4) . This also app­lies to a lar­ge extent to the male par­ti­ci­pants (61.4%), who, howe­ver, in con­trast to the girls and women, often name online games (28.9%). Over­all, the pre­fe­ren­ces dif­fer signi­fi­cant­ly from each other (<0.001).

Table 4: First men­ti­on of the main acti­vi­ties on the Inter­net among 14–24 year olds with a con­spi­cuous CIUS result (28 or more points) by gender


Acti­vi­ties online

Fre­quen­cy (%)

Con­fi­dence interval

Femi­ni­ne

Social net­works

81,4

64,4–91,4

E‑Mail

12,7

4,7–30,2

Online games

3,8

3,3–17,7

Enter­tain­ment (music, films, etc.)

2,1

0,3–14,3

Mas­cu­li­ne

Social net­works

61,4

43,5–76,7

Online games

28,9

15,7–47,1

Infor­ma­ti­on

3,5

0,4–23,0

E‑Mail

2,5

0,4–15,6

Shop­ping / Selling

2,4

0,3–16,8

Inter­net telephony

1,2

0,1–9,0

4.2 Prevalence estimate based on the latent class analysis (LCA) ‌

Latent class models with 2 to 7 clas­ses were cal­cu­la­ted. The models with 5 and 6 clas­ses show­ed an iden­ti­cal­ly lar­ge group that show­ed extre­me values in the CIUS. Com­pa­red to the 5‑class solu­ti­on, the 6‑class solu­ti­on show­ed the bet­ter model adap­t­ati­on (BIC: 22417 vs. 22490; Entro­py 0.769 vs. 0.762). The 6 groups do not over­lap, as can be seen in Figu­re 2.

Figu­re 2: Box plot of the CIUS total values for the 6 classes

A fur­ther ana­ly­sis of the 6‑class solu­ti­on reve­a­led fea­tures that speak for the exis­tence of a group that can be view­ed as depen­dent (class 6). A second group (class 5) pro­bab­ly shows an incre­a­sed risk in terms of pro­ble­ma­tic Inter­net use. The cor­re­spon­ding fin­dings are named below: This shows that class 6 has hig­her CIUS values. This can also be obser­ved to a les­ser extent for class 5 (Table 5). The same app­lies to the num­ber of hours that are spent on the Inter­net during the week. Over­all, gra­de 6 shows a lower level of social acti­vi­ties and social trust. Gra­de 5 is the youn­gest of the groups, fol­lo­wed by Gra­de 6.

Table 5: Cha­rac­te­ris­tics of the 6 clas­ses: mean values (stan­dard error)

Class

CIUS-

Average

Hours in the internet

/Week

Social

par­ti­ci­pa­ti­on

Social

Trust

Age

1

15,4 (0,03)

8,7 (0,19)

5,7 (0,07)

2,6 (0,02)

40,4 (0,34)

2

19,4 (0,03)

11,4 (0,34)

5,8 (0,74)

2,7 (0,02)

36,8 (0,44)

3

23,7 (0,03)

13,3 (0,33)

5,7 (0,73)

2,7 (0,02)

33,8 (0,41)

4

29, 6 (0,06)

16,0 (0,42)

5,5 (0,08)

2,7 (0,02)

31,1 (0,51)

5

37,3 (0,13)

22,5 (0,69)

5,3 (0,13)

2,7 (0,03)

27,6 (0,47)

6

48,7 (0,53)

29,2 (1,64)

5,0 (0,21)

2,5 (0,63)

30,0 (1,08)

signi­fi­can­ce

℗*

<0,001

<0,001

<0,001

<0,001

<0,001

* ANOVA (CIUS and age) or Krus­kal-Wal­lis‑H test based on unweigh­ted data

In the fol­lowing, the occur­rence of class 6 in the total popu­la­ti­on and in par­ti­al sam­ples is used to esti­ma­te the prevalence.

This results in an esti­ma­ted pre­va­lence for pro­bable inter­net addic­tion of 1.0% for all par­ti­ci­pants in the total sam­ple of 14 to 64 year olds. Fin­dings by gen­der and the respec­ti­ve con­fi­dence inter­vals can be found in Table 6.

Table 6: Pre­va­lence esti­ma­te of inter­net addic­tion based on the LCA (fre­quen­cy of class 6), ages 14–64 (n = 15,023)


Pre­va­lence

(%)

Con­fi­dence interval

(%)

Total

1,0

0,9–1,2

women

0,8

0,6–1,1

men

1,2

1,0–1,6

If the youn­ger age group is con­si­de­red sepa­r­ate­ly, this approach also results in hig­her pre­va­lence rates and the pre­va­lence levels are initi­al­ly equal with regard to the sexes. Among 14 to 16 year olds, the pre­va­lence is hig­her in fema­le par­ti­ci­pants (Tables 7 and 8).

Table 7: Pre­va­lence esti­ma­te of inter­net addic­tion based on the LCA (fre­quen­cy of class 6), ages 14–24 (n = 2,937)


Pre­va­lence

(%)

Con­fi­dence interval

(%)

Total

2,4

1,9–3,1

women

2,4

1,6–3,5

men

2,5

1,7–3,5

Table 8: Pre­va­lence esti­ma­te of inter­net addic­tion based on the LCA (fre­quen­cy of class 6), ages 14–16 (n = 693)


Pre­va­lence

(%)

Con­fi­dence interval

(%)

Total

4,0

2,7–5,7

women

4,9

2,8–8,5

men

3,1

1,8–5,3

When loo­king at the first-men­tio­ned main acti­vi­ties on the Inter­net, it again beco­mes appa­rent that social net­works are in the fore­ground for both sexes, but that the­se are men­tio­ned even more fre­quent­ly for girls and women (Table 9). In con­trast, boys and men play online games much more often. Over­all, the pre­fe­ren­ces also dif­fer signi­fi­cant­ly from each other here (<0.001). Some of the acti­vi­ties of the male par­ti­ci­pants lis­ted in Table 4 no lon­ger appe­ar here. Die Schät­zung auf Basis der LCA führt somit zu einer stär­ke­ren Fokus­sie­rung auf die Haupt­ak­ti­vi­tä­ten Sozia­le Netz­wer­ke und Online­spie­len, was zusätz­lich für die Vali­di­ät des LCA-Ansat­zes spre­chen mag.

Table 9: First men­ti­on of the main acti­vi­ties on the Inter­net of 14–24 year-olds in class 6 of the LCA by gender


Acti­vi­ties online

Fre­quen­cy (%)

Con­fi­dence interval

Femi­ni­ne

Social net­works

77,1

52,8–91,0

E‑Mail

11,7

2,7–39,3

Online games

7,2

1,5–28,3

Enter­tain­ment (music, films, etc.)

4,0

0,5–24,5

Mas­cu­li­ne

Social net­works

64,8

9,1–27,4

Online games

33,6

2,3–16,7

Inter­net telephony

1,5

0,5–1,3

In addi­ti­on to class 6, which can be view­ed as depen­dent, pre­va­lence rates can be given for class 5, which pres­um­a­b­ly has pro­ble­ma­tic Inter­net use. The cor­re­spon­ding data can be found in Tables 10 to 12. Over­all, the respec­ti­ve pro­por­ti­ons are signi­fi­cant­ly hig­her than in the case of the depen­den­cy. The­re are again hig­her rates in the youn­ger sam­ples and the pre­do­mi­nan­ce of women in the youn­ger age groups.

Table 10: Pre­va­lence esti­ma­te of pro­ble­ma­tic inter­net use based on the LCA (fre­quen­cy of class 5), ages 14–64 (n = 15,023)


Pre­va­lence

(%)

Con­fi­dence interval

(%)

Total

4,6

4,2–5,1

women

4,4

3,9–5,0

men

4,9

4,3–5,5

Table 11: Pre­va­lence esti­ma­te of pro­ble­ma­tic inter­net use based on the LCA (fre­quen­cy of class 5), ages 14–24 (n = 2,937)


Pre­va­lence

(%)

Con­fi­dence interval

(%)

Total

13,6

12,4–14,8

women

14,8

13,0–16,8

men

12,4

10,4–14,7

Table 12: Pre­va­lence esti­ma­te of pro­ble­ma­tic inter­net use based on the LCA (fre­quen­cy of class 5), ages 14–16 (n = 693)


Pre­va­lence

(%)

Con­fi­dence interval

(%)

Total

15,4

12,8–18,5

women

17,2

13,2–22,2

men

13,7

10,5–17,7

5. Discussion of the results‌

The pre­va­lence esti­ma­tes found in the PINTA stu­dy are below the pre­vious­ly avail­ab­le data from Hahn and Jeru­sa­lem (2001), who found a rate of 3.2% on the basis of an occa­sio­nal sam­ple in an online sur­vey. The esti­ma­tes of the pre­sent sam­ple are bet­ween 1% and 1.5%. The hig­her value was found using a cut-off from ano­t­her stu­dy (Van Rooij et al., 2011). This esti­ma­te has the fol­lowing sources of error: The com­pa­ra­ti­ve stu­dy is a sam­ple that is limi­ted to 13- to 16-year-old stu­dents. Fur­ther­mo­re, it was about the record­ing of online game addic­tion. The trans­fe­ra­bi­li­ty is the­re­fo­re limi­ted. In addi­ti­on, a pre­va­lence esti­ma­te based on a scree­ning pro­cess is always asso­cia­ted with a high error rate. Signi­fi­cant over­esti­ma­ti­ons can be made, par­ti­cu­lar­ly with low pre­va­lence and low spe­ci­fi­ci­ty (Gam­bi­no, 1997). Cor­rec­tions using for­mu­las that take sen­si­ti­vi­ty and spe­ci­fi­ci­ty into account, as ori­gi­nal­ly plan­ned, were not pos­si­ble in this case. The rea­son is that the­se two vali­di­ty mea­su­res are based on respon­se pro­ba­bi­li­ties of the LCA in the stu­dy by Van Rooij et al. could have been cal­cu­la­ted, but it would have been a cir­cu­lar argu­ment, sin­ce the con­spi­cuous class was also cal­cu­la­ted from the same ana­ly­sis. An exter­nal cri­ter­ion is missing.

The second approach of the pre­sent stu­dy was able to iden­ti­fy a class based on the LCA, which is very likely to repre­sent a group of inter­net addicts. This is sup­por­ted by a num­ber of fin­dings that dis­tin­guish it from the other clas­ses. This group had the hig­hest values in the CIUS, spent most of the time on the Internet,

show­ed less social acti­vi­ty, felt less social trust and was more likely to be young. A second group also stood out and can be view­ed as pro­ble­ma­tic with regard to Inter­net beha­vi­or. This pro­ce­du­re has the fol­lowing sources of error: 1. The group for­ma­ti­on is based sole­ly on the CIUS. Fea­tures that are not inclu­ded here are not taken into account. 2. Even if a num­ber of cri­te­ria have been used to find the most sui­ta­ble model, the­re is always some room for inter­pre­ta­ti­on. Taken tog­e­ther, the­re is no exter­nal vali­da­ti­on here either.

Over­all, it is assu­med that the esti­ma­te based on the LCA is clo­ser to the true pre­va­lence, sin­ce the sources of error in the other esti­ma­te must gene­ral­ly be asses­sed as signi­fi­cant­ly lar­ger. For the group as a who­le, the esti­ma­tes are also far apart.

In rela­ti­on to the pro­ject objec­ti­ves, it has clear­ly been pos­si­ble to make a more pre­cise esti­ma­te of the pre­va­lence pos­si­ble. The clear advan­ta­ge is the basis of a lar­ge and repre­sen­ta­ti­ve sam­ple which, in addi­ti­on to the fixed net­work sam­ple, also inclu­des peop­le who can only be reached via mobi­le phones.

When loo­king at the age groups and the dis­tri­bu­ti­on wit­hin the sexes, it is noti­ce­ab­le that in the young age groups the pre­va­lence rates of girls exceed tho­se of boys. Com­pa­red with ear­lier fin­dings (Hahn & Jeru­sa­lem, 2001; Peter­sen et al., 2010) this was not to be expec­ted. The fin­ding is all the more striking becau­se this trend was found in both esti­ma­tes using the dif­fe­rent metho­do­lo­gi­cal approa­ches. This can also be found in the LCA for the second con­spi­cuous class, who­se Inter­net use can be view­ed as pro­ble­ma­tic. If one loo­ks at the respec­ti­ve con­spi­cuous in the group of 14 to 24 year olds, one finds dif­fe­ren­ces in the pre­fe­ren­ces of the Inter­net acti­vi­ties. It is true that both groups most fre­quent­ly sta­te that social net­works are used, but this is par­ti­cu­lar­ly pro­noun­ced among women, who, on the other hand, rare­ly use online games. Over­all, and espe­cial­ly for the­se unex­pec­ted fin­dings in the young fema­le test per­sons, future stu­dies will have to cla­ri­fy whe­ther the abnor­ma­li­ties found are actual­ly to be unders­tood as a dis­or­der for which help is nee­ded. For this it is necessa­ry to con­duct in-depth inter­views that cap­tu­re the cli­ni­cal signi­fi­can­ce on the level of sym­ptoms and cri­te­ria as well as the asso­cia­ted impairments.

6. Gender mainstreaming aspects‌

Due to the repre­sen­ta­ti­ve­ness of the sam­ple and the sepa­ra­te ana­ly­zes for women and men, aspects of gen­der main­strea­ming could be ful­ly taken into account.

7. Overall assessment‌

The pro­ject objec­ti­ves were ful­ly achie­ved. In par­ti­cu­lar, the sta­tis­ti­cal­ly com­plex LCA could be used, which metho­do­lo­gi­cal­ly enab­led a more pre­cise esti­ma­te of the pre­va­lence. The delays in the pro­ject pro­cess due to this ana­ly­sis and the addi­ti­on of the sam­ple, which can only be reached via mobi­le pho­ne, can be clear­ly jus­ti­fied by the respec­ti­ve metho­do­lo­gi­cal gain.

8. Dissemination and publicity of the project results‌

Due to the very short dura­ti­on of the pro­ject, no broad publi­ca­ti­on acti­vi­ty has been pos­si­ble so far. This is plan­ned for the com­ing peri­od. The­re are alrea­dy plans for pre­sen­ta­ti­ons at the Addic­tion Con­gress in Frank­furt (Sep­tem­ber 28th-Octo­ber 1st, 2011) and at the Sci­en­ti­fic Dis­cus­sion of the Ger­man Socie­ty for Addic­tion Rese­arch and Addic­tion The­ra­py (DG-Sucht) in Lübeck (Decem­ber 2nd-4th, 2011). A pre­sen­ta­ti­on at the Cri­mi­no­lo­gi­cal Rese­arch Insti­tu­te in Hano­ver has alrea­dy taken place. Publi­ca­ti­ons in spe­cia­list jour­nals are to follow.

9. Utilization of the project results (sustainability / transfer potential) ‌

The results indi­ca­te high rates of pro­ble­ma­tic or addic­ti­ve inter­net use in young age groups, espe­cial­ly among women. In order to be able to assess whe­ther the­re is a par­ti­cu­lar need for pre­ven­ti­on or tre­at­ment offers, fur­ther cla­ri­fi­ca­ti­on of the­se initi­al fin­dings in a detail­ed fol­low-up stu­dy is urgent­ly required.

10. List of publications lectures‌

Rumpf, H. J., Mey­er, C. & John, U. (2011). Pre­va­lence of inter­net addic­tion (PINTA), Cri­mi­no­lo­gi­cal Rese­arch Insti­tu­te Lower Sax­o­ny. Han­no­ver, 09.05.2011.Rumpf, H. J., Mey­er, C. & John, U. (2011). Pre­va­lence of Inter­net Addic­tion (PINTA): Results and Out­look, Federal Minis­try of Health. Ber­lin, 07.04.2011.

11. Literature‌

Byun, S., Ruf­fi­ni, C., Mills, J. E., Dou­glas, A. C., Niang, M., Step­chen­ko­va, S., Lee, S. K., Lout­fi, J., Lee, J. K., Atal­lah, M. & Blan­ton, M. (2009). Inter­net addic­tion: meta­syn­the­sis of 1996–2006 quan­ti­ta­ti­ve rese­arch. Cyber­psy­chol Behav, 12, 203–207.

Chris­ta­kis, D. A. (2010). Inter­net addic­tion: a 21(st) cen­tu­ry epi­de­mic? Bmc Medi­ci­ne, 8, 3. Gam­bi­no, B. (1997). The cor­rec­tion for bias in pre­va­lence esti­ma­ti­on with scree­ning tests.

Jour­nal of Gamb­ling Stu­dies, 13, 343–351.

Hahn, A. & Jeru­sa­lem, M. (2001). Inter­net­sucht: Jugend­li­che gefan­gen im Netz. In J. Raithel (Ed.), Risi­ko­ver­hal­ten Jugend­li­cher: Erklä­run­gen, For­men und Prä­ven­ti­on (pp. 279- 293). Ber­lin: Les­ke + Budrich.

Han­son, B. S., Öster­gren, P. O., Elm­stahl, S., Isaacs­son, S. O. & Ran­s­tam, J. (1997). Relia­bi­li­ty and vali­di­ty assess­ments of mea­su­res od social net­works, social sup­port and con­trol — results from the Mal­mö Shoul­der and Neck Stu­dy. Scan­di­na­vi­an Jour­nal of Public Health, 25, 249–257.

Hess, D., Stein­we­de, A., Gil­berg, R. & Kleud­gen, M. (2011). Metho­den­be­richt PAGE — Patho­lo­gi­sches Glücks­spie­len und Epi­de­mio­lo­gie. Bonn: infas Insti­tut für ange­wand­te Sozi­al­wis­sen­schaf­ten GmbH.

Meer­kerk, G. J., Van Den Eijn­den, R., Ver­mulst, A. A. & Gar­ret­sen, H. F. L. (2009). The Com­pul­si­ve Inter­net Use Sca­le (CIUS): Some Psy­cho­metric Pro­per­ties. Cyber­psy­cho­lo­gy & Beha­vi­or, 12, 1–6.

Meix­ner, S. (2010). Exzes­si­ve Inter­net­nut­zung im Jugend­al­ter. Kin­der und Jugend­schutz in Wis­sen­schaft und Pra­xis, 55, 3–7.

Mey­er, C., Rumpf, H.-J., Kreu­zer, A.-., de Bri­to, S., Glo­ri­us, S., Jes­ke, C., Kas­tir­ke, N., Porz,

S., Schön, D., West­ram, A., Klin­ger, D., Goe­ze, D., Bischof, G. & John, U. (2011). Patho­lo­gi­sches Glücks­spie­len und Epi­de­mio­lo­gie (PAGE): Ent­ste­hung, Komor­bi­di­tät, Remis­si­on und Behand­lung. End­be­richt an das Hes­si­sche Minis­te­ri­um des Innern und für Sport. Uni­ver­si­tä­ten Greifs­wald und Lübeck. Retrie­ved, from the World Wide Web:

Muthén, L. K. & Muthén, B. O. (1998). Mplus User’s gui­de, 5th edn. Los Ange­les: Muthén & Muthén.

Peter­sen, K. U., Tho­ma­si­us, R., Schelb, Y., Spie­les, H., Traut­mann, S., Thiel, R. & Wey- mann, N. (2010). Bera­tungs- und Behand­lungs­an­ge­bo­te zum patho­lo­gi­schen Inter­net­ge­brauch in Deutsch­land. End­be­richt an das Bun­des­mi­nis­te­ri­um für Gesund­heit (BMG). Ham­burg: Uni­ver­si­täts­kli­ni­kum Ham­burg-Eppen­dorf, Deut­sches Zen­trum für Sucht­fra­gen des Kin­des- und Jugend­al­ters (DZSKJ).

Reh­bein, F., Klei­mann, M. & Mos­s­le, T. (2010). Pre­va­lence and risk fac­tors of video game depen­den­cy in ado­lescence: results of a Ger­man nati­on­wi­de sur­vey. Cyber­psy­chol Behav Soc Netw, 13, 269–277.

Van Rooij, A. J., Schoe­n­ma­kers, T. M., Ver­mulst, A. A., Van Den Eijn­den, R. J. & Van De Mheen, D. (2011). Online video game addic­tion: iden­ti­fi­ca­ti­on of addic­ted ado­lescent gamers. Addic­tion, 106, 205–212.

WHO. (2009). The World Men­tal Health Sur­vey Initia­ti­ve. Com­pu­ter Assis­ted Per­so­nal Inter­view (CAPI V21.1.1). Gamb­ling sec­tion. URL: http://www.hcp.med.harvard.edu/wmhcidi/instruments_capi.php.

Leave a Reply